3.3
Impact Factor

Original Research ARTICLE

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 29 June 2015 | http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00165

Oxytocin affects spontaneous neural oscillations in trauma-exposed war veterans

  • 1The Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel
  • 2Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel
  • 3Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Hadassah, Hebrew University, Ramat-Gan, Israel

Exposure to combat-related trauma often leads to lifetime functional impairments. Previous research demonstrated the effects of oxytocin (OT) administration on brain regions implicated in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD); yet OT’s effects on brain patterns in trauma-exposed veterans have not been studied. In the current study the effects of OT on spontaneous brain oscillatory activity were measured in 43 veterans using magnetoencephalography (MEG): 28 veterans who were exposed to a combat-related trauma and 15 trauma-unexposed controls. Participants participated in two experimental sessions and were administered OT or placebo (PBO) in a double-blind, placebo-control, within-subject design. Following OT/PBO administration, participants underwent a whole-head MEG scan. Plasma and salivary OT levels were assessed each session. Spontaneous brain activity measured during a 2-min resting period was subjected to source-localization analysis. Trauma-exposed veterans showed higher resting-state alpha (8–13 Hz) activity compared to controls in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), specifically in the superior frontal gyrus (SFG) and the middle frontal gyrus (MFG), indicating decreased neural activity in these regions. The higher alpha activity was “normalized” following OT administration and under OT, group differences were no longer found. Increased resting-state alpha was associated with lower baseline plasma OT, reduced salivary OT reactivity, and more re-experiencing symptoms. These findings demonstrate effects of OT on resting-state brain functioning in prefrontal regions subserving working memory and cognitive control, which are disrupted in PTSD. Results raise the possibility that OT, traditionally studied in social contexts, may also enhance performance in cognitive tasks associated with working memory and cognitive control following trauma exposure.

Introduction

Combat-related post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a debilitating disorder that often leads to lifetime impairments in cognitive processing, social relationships, and emotion regulation (Yehuda, 2002; American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Prevalence studies have shown that between 2 and 17% of US veterans develop PTSD (Richardson et al., 2010), particularly those who witnessed injury or death of another person (Smith et al., 2008). Furthermore, many veterans show substantial functional impairment, work and family problems, and dramatic reduction in quality of life even decades after combat (Schnurr et al., 2009). These are also observed in veterans who do not meet full criteria for PTSD (Schnurr et al., 2000; Grubaugh et al., 2005; Jakupcak et al., 2007).

Research has shown significant alterations in brain structure and function in cases of PTSD in a number of regions, including the amygdala and the prefrontal cortex (PFC; Karl et al., 2006; Hayes et al., 2012; Pitman et al., 2012). Functional neuroimaging studies using different paradigms, such as symptom provocation or cognitive and emotional tasks, have demonstrated hyper-activation of the amygdala (Liberzon et al., 1999; Rauch et al., 2000; Bryant et al., 2005; Fonzo et al., 2010; Simmons et al., 2011) and hypo-activation of the PFC (Shin et al., 2001; Lanius et al., 2003; Williams et al., 2006; Milad et al., 2009), and some have shown inversed relation of activity (Shin et al., 2004, 2005) and abnormal connectivity (Stevens et al., 2013) between the amygdala and the PFC. Comparable findings were also observed during a resting-state, i.e., in the absence of task demands (Chung et al., 2006; Sripada et al., 2012; Yan et al., 2013). It has been suggested that amygdala hyper-responsivity and impaired top-down prefrontal control mediate the dysregulation of fear and threat-related processing in PTSD (Rauch et al., 2006). More recently it has been proposed (Patel et al., 2012) that abnormalities in three different neuro-cognitive networks may explain the range of symptoms seen in PTSD. One of these networks, the central executive network (Menon, 2011), is associated with high-level cognitive functions, such as decision making, attention control, and working memory, and is anchored in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), a region critical to proper cognitive control over emotions (Ochsner et al., 2002; Phan et al., 2005). Thus, processes typical of PTSD such as negative appraisals of stimuli, as well as the disrupted cognitive functioning seen in PTSD, may be related to aberrant top-down signaling from this network (Patel et al., 2012).

Decreased activity in the dlPFC has been observed in PTSD during the performance of cognitive tasks (Clark et al., 2003; Falconer et al., 2008; Rabinak et al., 2014) and during a resting-state (Yan et al., 2013) and such decrease is associated with PTSD symptom severity (Falconer et al., 2008; Yan et al., 2013). Studies assessing the effects of therapy on brain function in PTSD have shown that positive effects were associated with activation in frontal regions, including the dlPFC (Cohen et al., 2004; Farrow et al., 2005; Peres et al., 2007; Lindauer et al., 2008; Boggio et al., 2010; Watts et al., 2012; Thomaes et al., 2014). These studies suggest that the dlPFC may be a target region for intervention effort in PTSD and that improved functioning in this region may serve as an index of positive therapeutic effects.

Intranasal administration of oxytocin (OT), a nine-amino-acid neuropeptide implicated in mammalian caregiving and sociality (Feldman, 2012; Carter, 2014), has been shown to exert regulatory effects on brain patterns in healthy individuals exposed to threatening stimuli and in individuals with a variety of psychiatric conditions due to its anxiolytic properties (Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2011; Bethlehem et al., 2013; Weisman and Feldman, 2013; MacDonald and Feifel, 2014). The anxiolytic and stress protective effects of OT have been demonstrated in animal (Windle et al., 2004; Blume et al., 2008; Viviani et al., 2011; Peters et al., 2014) and human (Heinrichs et al., 2003; Light et al., 2005; Gordon et al., 2008; Ditzen et al., 2009) research. In animals, research has shown that the induction of endogenous OT release from the central amygdala decreases fear responses (Knobloch et al., 2012). In humans, anxiolytic effects have been demonstrated by the attenuation of amygdala activation to fearful stimuli following OT administration, as observed in functional MRI studies with healthy adults (Kirsch et al., 2005; Domes et al., 2007). In addition, OT has been shown to increase resting-state functional connectivity between the amygdala and frontal regions that are critical for fear extinction and regulation of emotions (Sripada et al., 2013), further supporting its anti-stress effects. The anxiolytic and stress reducing properties of OT have been suggested as one mechanism that underpins the well-established pro-social effects of OT in humans (Churchland and Winkielman, 2012; MacDonald and Feifel, 2013).

Studies utilizing intranasal OT administration in clinical populations have shown that OT improved core symptoms and aberrant brain patterns in cases of schizophrenia (Feifel et al., 2010), autism (Domes et al., 2013; Gordon et al., 2013), and social anxiety (Labuschagne et al., 2010; Dodhia et al., 2014). Studies have also shown that OT administration improved symptom severity (Yatzkar and Klein, 2010) and reduced the physiological response to combat imagery (Pitman et al., 1993) in PTSD. The effects of OT administration on reducing the amygdala fear-response (Kirsch et al., 2005; Domes et al., 2007) and on increasing amygdala-prefrontal connectivity in healthy adults (Sripada et al., 2013) and in adults with generalized social anxiety disorder (Dodhia et al., 2014), suggest that OT may exert a regulatory function on brain patterns following exposure to combat-related trauma.

In light of the above, the current study examined the effects of OT administration on spontaneous (resting-state) brain activity in trauma-exposed war veterans. Spontaneous activity has been used to examine the intrinsic functional activity of the brain (Raichle and Mintun, 2006) and has been shown to index risk for various psychiatric disorders including PTSD (Buckner and Vincent, 2007). In PTSD, abnormal oscillatory activity in frontal regions and in the insula was found during resting-state in trauma-exposed individuals using magnetoencephalography (MEG; Kolassa et al., 2007). In another MEG study, resting-state activity differentiated patients from controls, suggesting that spontaneous brain patterns may serve as neuromarkers for the disorder (Georgopoulos et al., 2010). Brain oscillations reflect synchronized activity of large populations of neurons and support normal brain function (Buzsáki and Draguhn, 2004). In the current study, we were particularly interested in alpha rhythm, the predominant oscillatory frequency in humans during eyes-closed rest (Nunez et al., 2001). Alpha activity has been associated with decreased cortical activity (Goldman et al., 2002; Laufs et al., 2003a; Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010), and anomalies in resting-state alpha have been observed in various psychiatric disorders (Ciesielski et al., 2007; Hinkley et al., 2011; Cornew et al., 2012), including combat-related PTSD (Jokić-begić and Begić, 2003; Huang et al., 2014).

We further measured whether post-traumatic symptoms and peripheral OT levels may be associated with the effects of OT administration on spontaneous oscillatory brain patterns.

Materials and Methods

Participants

At the first stage of the study, 190 young male veterans (age < 35 years) were recruited. These included 158 veterans who served in the Israel Defense Force (IDF) in active combat units during the past 8 years, participated in active battle that involved life endangerment, and witnessed injury or death of a comrade. Thirty-two veterans matched for age and education, who served in the IDF during the same period in non-combat units (e.g., intelligence, technical support) and were not exposed to active combat were recruited as controls. All veterans completed the Post-Traumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale (PDS; Foa et al., 1997) for the assessment of post-traumatic symptom severity and in order to ensure that members of the control group were free of post traumatic symptoms. Twenty-eight veterans from the trauma-exposed group with non-zero PDS scores (mean age = 27 years, SD = 1.81) and 16 controls with 0 PDS scores (mean age = 28.94 years, SD = 3.02) who met inclusion criteria participated in the OT administration and MEG study, as described below. Most members of the trauma-exposed group experienced the traumatic event 3 or 5 years before the study, during two distinct military operations, and the others in various other operations in the period between 3 to 8 years before the study.

Participants were recruited via advertisement in the community and were matched for demographic status. Subjects were right-handed as measured by the Edinburgh Handedness Questionnaire except for three subjects from the exposed group who were neutral (N = 1) or left handed (N = 2). Exclusion criteria included serious physical injury, current or past neurological disorders, serious medical problems, and regular use of medication. War-exposed veterans were not exposed to trauma other than combat. Control veterans were not exposed to trauma of any kind and had no present psychiatric disorder. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Bar-Ilan University and after complete description of the study, written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Experimental Procedure

Clinical Diagnosis

All participants were diagnosed by clinical psychologists blind to trauma history supervised by a psychiatrist using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I; First et al., 1997). Among controls, none received any Axis-I psychiatric diagnosis. Among trauma-exposed veterans 11 were diagnosed with PTSD and five of them showed co-morbidity which included depression (N = 3), dysthymia (N =‘2), phobia (N = 2), panic attacks (N = 1), and OCD (N =‘2). Trauma-exposed veterans who did not meet criteria for a full PTSD diagnosis showed no difference in their post-traumatic symptom scores in any symptom cluster (re-experience, avoidance, hyper-arousal) or in the functional impairment domain on the PDS. Mean PDS symptom severity scores were 19.72 (SD = 9.12) for trauma-exposed veterans who met DSM-IV criteria for a full PTSD Axis-I diagnosis and 16.13 (SD = 7.22) for trauma-exposed veterans who did not meet criteria for full PTSD diagnosis (F(1,25) = 1.25, NS). We therefore combined the two groups into a single group. This is consistent with the DSM-V approach, which advocates a dimensional approach to psychiatric disorders as indexed by symptom severity profiles (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

Experimental Design

The study employed a double-blind, placebo (PBO)-control, within-subject design. Each subject participated in two similar experimental sessions approximately a week apart (mean = 7.5 days, SD = 2). Baseline (T1) blood and saliva samples were collected in each session. Following, participants self-administered 24 IU of either OT (Syntocinon Spray, Novartis, Switzerland; three puffs per nostril, each containing 4 IU) or PBO. PBO was custom-designed by commercial compounding pharmacy to match drug minus the active ingredient. Administration order was counterbalanced, and participants and experimenters were blind to drug condition. Experimental sessions were conducted between 14:30 and 16:00 pm to accommodate diurnal variations in OT.

Forty-five minutes after OT/PBO administration, spontaneous brain activity was measured using MEG during a 2 min period when subjects rested with their eyes closed. Two additional saliva samples were collected in each experimental session, one before (T2) and one after (T3) the MEG scanning.

Brain Activity Recording and Analysis

MEG recordings were conducted with a whole-head, 248-channel magnetometer array (4-D Neuroimaging, Magnes 3600 WH) in a magnetically-shielded room with a sample rate of 1017 Hz and online 1–400 Hz band-pass filter in a supine position. External noise (e.g., power-line, mechanical vibrations) and heartbeat artifacts were removed from the data as previously described (Tal and Abeles, 2013). Signal pre-processing at the sensor level was carried out using MATLAB and the FieldTrip toolbox (Oostenveld et al., 2011). The data were segmented into 1000 ms epochs, filtered in the 1–80 Hz range with 10 s padding and were baseline-corrected. The data epochs were visually inspected for eye movement and muscle artifacts which were excluded from further analysis.

For source estimation, a template MRI (Collin27) was modified to fit each subject’s digitized head shape using SPM8 (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, University College London, London, UK).1 Synthetic aperture magnetometry (SAM) beamformer (Robinson and Vrba, 1999) was applied with a spatial resolution of 0.5 cm for each experimental condition (OT or PBO) at four frequency bands: delta (1–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–13 Hz) and beta (13–20 Hz). Individual source power maps were reconstructed using pseudo-Z statistic. Following transformation into a common coordinate space (Talairach) clusters with significant effects were identified by a voxel-level t-test and were corrected for multiple comparisons based on a Monte Carlo simulation of random noise distribution (Forman et al., 1995). The results were superimposed on the MRI template as statistical maps, showing regions of statistically significant difference in activity. The voxel average activity in clusters with significant group differences at baseline (under PBO) was used in the following analyses.

Oxytocin Collection and Determination

Blood was drawn at baseline, before OT/PBO administration, during each visit from antecubital veins into nine milliliter chilled vacutainer tubes containing lithium heparin supplemented with 400 KIU of Trasylol (Trasylol–Bayer, Germany) per one milliliter blood. Blood samples were kept ice-chilled for up to 2 h before centrifuged at 4°C at 1000 × g for 15 min. Supernatants were collected and stored at −80°C until assayed.

Saliva samples were collected three times each visit as described above. OT from saliva was collected by Salivate (Sarstedt, Rommelsdorft, Germany). Salivates were kept ice-chilled for up to 1 h before centrifuged at 4°C at 1500 × g for 15 min. Liquid samples were stored at −80°C. To concentrate the samples by 3–4 times, liquid samples were lyophilized over-night and kept in −20°C until assayed. Dry samples were reconstructed in the assay buffer immediately before analysis by OT EIA commercial Kit, consistent with previous research (Feldman et al., 2010).

Determination of OT was performed using commercial ELISA kit (Assay Design, MI, USA; through ENZO, NY, USA), consistent with our previous research (Feldman et al., 2011; Weisman et al., 2013). Measurements were performed in duplicate and concentration of samples calculated using MATLAB according to relevant standard curves. The intra-assay and inter-assay coefficients were <12.3 and <14.5%, respectively.

Immunoassay methods (EIA/ELISA) for the quantitative determination of OT and is considered the gold standard in current OT research and has shown to be sensitive and reliable (Kramer et al., 2004), and unlike the earlier radioimmunoassay (RIA) method it does not involve the use of radioactive substances (but note several methodological issues, see “Limitations” Section).

Two measures of peripheral OT were computed:

Baseline plasma OT was the average of baseline (T1) OT levels in the two visits, which were highly correlated, (r = 0.97, p < 0.0001).

Salivary OT reactivity was measured as the increase in OT from baseline (T1) after OT administration (T2 + T3 2 minus T1 in the OT session).

Results

Group Differences in Spontaneous Brain Activity Under Placebo

Analysis was first performed for the PBO condition in order to assess group differences in spontaneous brain activity at baseline. Group differences were found only in alpha-band activity (8–13 Hz), located in a cluster of two regions within the left dlPFC (Figure 1A): the left superior frontal gyrus (SFG) and the left middle frontal gyrus (MFG). In these regions, the trauma-exposed group showed increased alpha activity compared to the control group (Figure 1B; F(1,40) = 6.88, p = 0.014, and F(1,40) = 5.82, p = 0.021, for SFG and MFG, respectively).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Between-group differences in spontaneous brain activity at baseline (placebo). (A) Dorsolateral prefrontal cluster of the left superior frontal gyrus (SFG) and the left middle frontal gyrus (MFG) showing higher alpha activity in the trauma-exposed group compared to the control group, p < 0.05, corrected. x, y, z of maximal voxels in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates = −17, 59, 22 and −27, 64, 27, for SFG and MFG, respectively. Averaged alpha activity over all voxels in the SFG and in the MFG (B). *p < 0.05. Error bars represent SEM.

Effects of Oxytocin on Spontaneous Brain Activity

In the next stage, we tested the effect of OT administration on alpha activity in the two dlPFC regions that showed group differences at PBO, the left SFG and the left MFG. A repeated measure analysis of variance (ANOVA) showed a significant interaction effect of group and OT administration for both the SFG (F(1,38) = 5.76, p = 0.021), and the MFG (F(1,38) = 4.89, p = 0.033), indicating that OT’s effect differed by group (Figure 2). Specifically, OT administration significantly decreased the heightened alpha activity among trauma-exposed veterans, and under OT, group differences were no longer found. In the control group however, OT increased the alpha activity, but this finding was not statistically significant.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The effects of oxytocin (OT) administration on spontaneous alpha activity. Averaged alpha activity over all voxels in the left SFG and in the left MFG in the placebo and OT conditions. *p < 0.05. Error bars represent SEM.

Peripheral Oxytocin

No differences were found between groups in plasma and salivary OT levels at baseline or in OT reactivity.

Spontaneous Alpha Activity, Peripheral Oxytocin and Symptom Severity

Alpha activity at baseline (at PBO) correlated with peripheral measures of OT for the entire sample and with symptom severity in the trauma-exposed group. Higher alpha in the left SFG was associated with lower levels of baseline plasma OT (r = −0.38, p = 0.014), and higher alpha in the left MFG was associated with lower salivary OT reactivity (r = −0.35, p = 0.025). Interestingly, more re-experiencing symptoms correlated with higher baseline alpha in both the left SFG, (r = 0.43, p = 0.027), and the left MFG, (r = 0.39, p = 0.045). Re-experiencing symptoms index the uncontrolled re-living of the trauma in the form of thoughts and images of the traumatic event, nightmares, flashbacks, psychological distress and physiological reactivity to trauma-reminders.

Discussion

Results of the current study, the first to test the brain response of trauma-exposed veterans to OT administration, demonstrate that OT significantly attenuated the aberrant alpha activity in dorsolateral prefrontal regions implicated in working memory and cognitive control, in trauma-exposed veterans. Spontaneous oscillations in the alpha range are the predominant electrophysiological signal in healthy adults during rest when the attention system is not recruited toward a specific task (Nunez et al., 2001). Alpha oscillations are associated with decreased cortical activity, are thought to reflect functional inhibition (Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010), and are negatively correlated with the strength of BOLD fMRI signal in frontal and parietal cortical areas that support attention and related cognitive processes (Laufs et al., 2003b). This suggests that higher alpha power provides an index of decreased cortical activity in these regions, and our findings suggest that OT may have some impact, albeit momentarily, on prefrontal structures suggested as among those underpinning the functional impairment in PTSD (Patel et al., 2012).

Specifically, we found that OT attenuated the increased alpha power observed among trauma-exposed veterans in two frontal regions, the left SFG and the left MFG. Neuroimaging and human lesion studies indicated that both the SFG and MFG are critical for working memory and cognitive control (Aron et al., 2004; Egner and Hirsch, 2005; du Boisgueheneuc et al., 2006; Jurado and Rosselli, 2007). In patients with left SFG lesion, impairments to working memory increased as the task demands became more complex (du Boisgueheneuc et al., 2006) and in patients with left MFG lesion (Aron et al., 2004) the degree of cognitive control impairments correlated with the extent of damage to this region. Studies have repeatedly shown dysfunction in working memory (Elzinga and Bremner, 2002), attention, and cognitive control (Banich et al., 2009) in patients with PTSD, including combat-related PTSD (Vasterling et al., 1998). Our findings, therefore, raise the possibility that OT, which has been studied almost entirely in the social context, may also act to enhance performance in cognitive tasks associated with working memory and cognitive control which are disrupted in PTSD or may be used in conjunction with cognitive-behavior therapy, but this hypothesis is only preliminary and requires much further systematic research in larger samples.

The difficulties seen in PTSD patients in working memory, attention, and cognitive control (Banich et al., 2009) have been shown to index PFC disturbances. Neuroimaging studies found hypo-activity of the PFC, hyper-activity of the amygdala (Hayes et al., 2012; Pitman et al., 2012), and inverse relation of activity in the amygdala and the PFC (Shin et al., 2004, 2005) in cases of PTSD. Abnormal activity in the central executive network, a brain network which is situated in the dlPFC and is associated with high-level cognitive functions (Menon, 2011), has been implicated in some of the behavioral difficulties in PTSD (Patel et al., 2012). The increase in activity in the left SFG and MFG following OT administration, indexed by the decrease in alpha activity found here, may suggest that OT exerts anxiolytic effects by enhancing activity in an attention-related network implicated in the application of cognitive, goal-directed selection to sensory information and responses (Egner and Hirsch, 2005). Furthermore, previous studies indicated that the SFG and MFG are sensitive to OT administration in patient populations. Among depressed individuals OT increased activity in the SFG and MFG (Pincus et al., 2010) and among children with autism OT increased activity in the MFG (Gordon et al., 2013). Our findings, therefore, are consistent with the current effort to specify the effects of OT on brain functioning in different psychiatric conditions (Weisman and Feldman, 2013).

Our findings are also in line with research demonstrating structural (Geuze et al., 2008; Tavanti et al., 2012) and functional (Chung et al., 2006; Morey et al., 2008) alterations in the SFG and MFG in patients with PTSD. Moreover, the degree of alterations in these regions was found to correlate with the severity of PTSD symptoms (Morey et al., 2008; Tavanti et al., 2012). Our data similarly show that the degree of SFG and MFG hypo-activity at baseline, indexed by increased alpha activity, correlated with the number of re-experiencing symptoms, including repeated flashbacks, sensory-based memories, and uncontrolled reliving of trauma. A negative correlation between spontaneous activity in the dlPFC and re-experiencing symptoms has been reported in a previous work (Yan et al., 2013) and it has been suggested to be related to the role of this region in inhibiting unwanted memories (Anderson et al., 2004; Yan et al., 2013). Additionally, research has shown that increased activity in SFG and MFG following cognitive behavior therapy was related to PTSD symptom improvement (Lindauer et al., 2008). Finally, evidence indicates that frontal alpha activity is sensitive to therapy in PTSD (Kluetsch et al., 2014). Taken together, these studies underscore the sensitivity of these dorsolateral prefrontal regions to both symptom severity and treatment effects in PTSD. This is in line with perspectives suggesting that the prefrontal deficits in PTSD are not innate but acquired following the onset of the disorder and may thus be more amenable to intervention (Admon et al., 2013).

We are aware of no prior study that examined the effects of OT administration on brain response in trauma-exposed combat veterans and the findings therefore suggest that much further research is warranted to fully test the potential beneficial effects of OT in this context. Our findings that OT administration was effective for some individuals but not for others is consistent with the general observation that OT’s effect is influenced by various personal factors (Bartz et al., 2011b), including the presence of psychiatric symptoms, as demonstrated in a number of studies showing differential effects of OT administration on brain activity among patients and controls (Labuschagne et al., 2010; Pincus et al., 2010; Dodhia et al., 2014). Such data strengthen the view of the complex nature of OT (e.g., Bartz et al., 2011b; Weisman and Feldman, 2013) and support the current call to implement personalized intervention that is guided by the individual’s symptomatology for targeted and effective treatments. Very little research examined the involvement of the OT system in PTSD and much further research is required. The observation that OT’s effects are influenced by personal as well as contextual factors (Bartz et al., 2011b) and importantly, are not always positive (e.g., Bartz et al., 2011a; De Dreu et al., 2011; Shalvi and De Dreu, 2014) emphasize the need to further examine its effects in PTSD, also in other contexts, using different experimental tasks, in addition to the currently used resting-state assessment.

Overall, the high number of returning veterans with traumatic war experiences and the vulnerability of this population to develop PTSD (Kessler et al., 1995; Richardson et al., 2010), underscore the importance and potential clinical utility of these findings, but, at the same time, emphasize the preliminary nature of our study. Much further research is required to describe the effects of OT on oscillatory brain activity at rest and in response to various tasks, to test mechanisms by which OT impacts other neurohormonal systems, such as the HPA, dopamine, or immune systems, and to specify the types of psychological interventions that may work in concert with OT to increase well-being among patients suffering from post-traumatic distress.

Limitations

Several study limitations should be remembered in the interpretation of the findings. First, although our study is large for a brain study, involving 86 brain scans—among the largest OT-administration brain studies—the number of veterans with PTSD is relatively small and the findings require further validation and specification before generalizability can be made. Second, we specifically chose veterans who are young, relatively functional, and free from medical illness, and further research is required to test whether our findings generalize to veterans with long-lasting PTSD, medical conditions, or functional impairment. Next, some methodological concerns regarding OT measurement should be considered. First, there is still uncertainty as to what other molecules are possibly being measured along with OT using the commonly applied methods for OT levels determination, (i.e., RIA vs. EIA), especially when pre-analysis extraction procedure is not included (McCullough et al., 2013), as was the case in our study. Further validation and standardization are needed to better understand the meaning of these measures. Second, while OT is released by the pituitary gland into the blood stream and is presumed to be the primary source of salivary OT (Carter et al., 2007), the relation between peripheral OT measures and OT’s function within the CNS is still not well understood; therefore, the physiological meaning of these measures should be interpreted with caution. Considering plasma vs. salivary OT measures, saliva sampling is advantageous since it is a convenient, non-invasive and less stressful procedure (Carter et al., 2007). However, the plasma measure is considered a more direct one, yet we found in several studies that plasma and saliva measures show medium-level correlations (Feldman et al., 2011). Finally, it is important to complement brain activity in the absence of task (i.e., “resting state”) with response patterns to specific tasks related to fear stimuli in general or to the individual’s specific traumatic memories for a fuller understanding of brain patterns underpinning PTSD and their response to oxytocin administration.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Supported by NARSAD independent investigator award and by the Simms-Mann Foundation.

Abbreviations

OT, Oxytocin; PBO, placebo; dlPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; SFG, superior frontal gyrus; MFG, middle frontal gyrus.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/

References

Admon, R., Milad, M. R., and Hendler, T. (2013). A causal model of post-traumatic stress disorder: disentangling predisposed from acquired neural abnormalities. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 337–347. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-V. 5th Edn. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Anderson, M. C., Ochsner, K. N., Kuhl, B., Cooper, J., Robertson, E., Gabrieli, S. W., et al. (2004). Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories. Science 303, 232–235. doi: 10.1126/science.1089504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A. R., Monsell, S., Sahakian, B. J., and Robbins, T. W. (2004). A componential analysis of task-switching deficits associated with lesions of left and right frontal cortex. Brain 127, 1561–1573. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banich, M. T., Mackiewicz, K. L., Depue, B. E., Whitmer, A., Miller, G. A., and Heller, W. (2009). Cognitive control mechanisms, emotion and memory: a neural perspective with implications for psychopathology. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 613–630. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartz, J., Simeon, D., Hamilton, H., Kim, S., Crystal, S., Braun, A., et al. (2011a). Oxytocin can hinder trust and cooperation in borderline personality disorder. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 6, 556–563. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartz, J. A., Zaki, J., Bolger, N., and Ochsner, K. N. (2011b). Social effects of oxytocin in humans: context and person matter. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 301–309. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bethlehem, R. A. I., van Honk, J., Auyeung, B., and Baron-Cohen, S. (2013). Oxytocin, brain physiology and functional connectivity: a review of intranasal oxytocin fMRI studies. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 962–974. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blume, A., Bosch, O. J., Miklos, S., Torner, L., Wales, L., Waldherr, M., et al. (2008). Oxytocin reduces anxiety via ERK1/2 activation: local effect within the rat hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus. Eur. J. Neurosci. 27, 1947–1956. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06184.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boggio, P. S., Rocha, M., Oliveira, M. O., Fecteau, S., Cohen, R. B., Campanhã, C., et al. (2010). Noninvasive brain stimulation with high-frequency and low-intensity repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation treatment for posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Clin. Psychiatry 71, 992–999. doi: 10.4088/jcp.08m04638blu

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryant, R. A., Felmingham, K. L., Kemp, A. H., Barton, M., Peduto, A. S., Rennie, C., et al. (2005). Neural networks of information processing in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol. Psychiatry 58, 111–118. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.03.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., and Vincent, J. L. (2007). Unrest at rest: default activity and spontaneous network correlations. Neuroimage 37, 1091–1096. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G., and Draguhn, A. (2004). Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science 304, 1926–1929. doi: 10.1126/science.1099745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, C. S., Pournajafi-Nazarloo, H., Kramer, K. M., Ziegler, T. E., White-Traut, R., Bello, D., et al. (2007). Oxytocin: behavioral associations and potential as a salivary biomarker. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1098, 312–322. doi: 10.1196/annals.1384.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, C. S. (2014). Oxytocin pathways and the evolution of human behavior. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 65, 17–39. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, Y. A., Kim, S. H., Chung, S. K., Chae, J.-H., Yang, D. W., Sohn, H. S., et al. (2006). Alterations in cerebral perfusion in posttraumatic stress disorder patients without re-exposure to accident-related stimuli. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 637–642. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.10.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Churchland, P. S., and Winkielman, P. (2012). Modulating social behavior with oxytocin: how does it work? What does it mean? Horm. Behav. 61, 392–399. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciesielski, K. T., Hämäläinen, M. S., Geller, D. A., Wilhelm, S., Goldsmith, T. E., and Ahlfors, S. P. (2007). Dissociation between MEG alpha modulation and performance accuracy on visual working memory task in obsessive compulsive disorder. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 1401–1414. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, C. R., McFarlane, A. C., Morris, P., Weber, D. L., Sonkkilla, C., Shaw, M., et al. (2003). Cerebral function in posttraumatic stress disorder during verbal working memory updating: a positron emission tomography study. Biol. Psychiatry 53, 474–481. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01505-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, H., Kaplan, Z., Kotler, M., Kouperman, I., Moisa, R., and Grisaru, N. (2004). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in posttraumatic stress disorder: a double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Am. J. Psychiatry 161, 515–524. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.3.515

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cornew, L., Roberts, T. P., Blaskey, L., and Edgar, J. C. (2012). Resting-state oscillatory activity in autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 1884–1894. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1431-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Dreu, C. K. W., Greer, L. L., Van Kleef, G. A., Shalvi, S., and Handgraaf, M. J. J. (2011). Oxytocin promotes human ethnocentrism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 108, 1262–1266. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015316108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ditzen, B., Schaer, M., Gabriel, B., Bodenmann, G., Ehlert, U., and Heinrichs, M. (2009). Intranasal oxytocin increases positive communication and reduces cortisol levels during couple conflict. Biol. Psychiatry 65, 728–731. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodhia, S., Hosanagar, A., Fitzgerald, D. A., Labuschagne, I., Wood, A. G., Nathan, P. J., et al. (2014). Modulation of resting-state amygdala-frontal functional connectivity by oxytocin in generalized social anxiety disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 39, 2061–2069. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.53

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domes, G., Heinrichs, M., Gläscher, J., Büchel, C., Braus, D. F., and Herpertz, S. C. (2007). Oxytocin attenuates amygdala responses to emotional faces regardless of valence. Biol. Psychiatry 62, 1187–1190. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.03.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domes, G., Heinrichs, M., Kumbier, E., Grossmann, A., Hauenstein, K., and Herpertz, S. C. (2013). Effects of intranasal oxytocin on the neural basis of face processing in autism spectrum disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 74, 164–171. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

du Boisgueheneuc, F., Levy, R., Volle, E., Seassau, M., Duffau, H., Kinkingnehun, S., et al. (2006). Functions of the left superior frontal gyrus in humans: a lesion study. Brain 129, 3315–3328. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egner, T., and Hirsch, J. (2005). The neural correlates and functional integration of cognitive control in a stroop task. Neuroimage 24, 539–547. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elzinga, B. M., and Bremner, J. D. (2002). Are the neural substrates of memory the final common pathway in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD)? J. Affect. Disord. 70, 1–17. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(01)00351-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falconer, E., Bryant, R., Felmingham, K. L., Kemp, A. H., Gordon, E., Peduto, A., et al. (2008). The neural networks of inhibitory control in posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 33, 413–422.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Farrow, T. F., Hunter, M. D., Wilkinson, I. D., Gouneea, C., Fawbert, D., Smith, R., et al. (2005). Quantifiable change in functional brain response to empathic and forgivability judgments with resolution of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res. 140, 45–53. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2005.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feifel, D., Macdonald, K., Nguyen, A., Cobb, P., Warlan, H., Galangue, B., et al. (2010). Adjunctive intranasal oxytocin reduces symptoms in schizophrenia patients. Biol. Psychiatry 68, 678–680. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.04.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R. (2012). Oxytocin and social affiliation in humans. Horm. Behav. 61, 380–391. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R., Gordon, I., Schneiderman, I., Weisman, O., and Zagoory-Sharon, O. (2010). Natural variations in maternal and paternal care are associated with systematic changes in oxytocin following parent-infant contact. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 1133–1141. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.01.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R., Gordon, I., and Zagoory-Sharon, O. (2011). Maternal and paternal plasma, salivary and urinary oxytocin and parent-infant synchrony: considering stress and affiliation components of human bonding. Dev. Sci. 14, 752–761. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01021.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., and Williams, J. B. (1997). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders, Clinician Version (SCID-CV). Wash. DC: Am. Psychiatr. Assoc.

Foa, E. B., Cashman, L., Jaycox, L., and Perry, K. (1997). The validation of a self-report measure of posttraumatic stress disorder: the Posttraumatic diagnostic scale. Psychol. Assess. 9, 445–451. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.9.4.445

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonzo, G. A., Simmons, A. N., Thorp, S. R., Norman, S. B., Paulus, M. P., and Stein, M. B. (2010). Exaggerated and disconnected insular-amygdalar blood oxygenation level-dependent response to threat-related emotional faces in women with intimate-partner violence posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 68, 433–441. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.04.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forman, S. D., Cohen, J. D., Fitzgerald, M., Eddy, W. F., Mintun, M. A., and Noll, D. C. (1995). Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster-size threshold. Magn. Reson. Med. 33, 636–647. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910330508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Georgopoulos, A. P., Tan, H. M., Lewis, S. M., Leuthold, A. C., Winskowski, A. M., Lynch, J. K., et al. (2010). The synchronous neural interactions test as a functional neuromarker for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD): a robust classification method based on the bootstrap. J. Neural Eng. 7:016011. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/7/1/016011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geuze, E., Westenberg, H. G. M., Heinecke, A., de Kloet, C. S., Goebel, R., and Vermetten, E. (2008). Thinner prefrontal cortex in veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuroimage 41, 675–681. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldman, R. I., Stern, J. M., Engel, J. Jr., and Cohen, M. S. (2002). Simultaneous EEG and fMRI of the alpha rhythm. Neuroreport 13, 2487–2492. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200212200-00022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gordon, I., Vander Wyk, B. C., Bennett, R. H., Cordeaux, C., Lucas, M. V., Eilbott, J. A., et al. (2013). Oxytocin enhances brain function in children with autism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 110, 20953–20958. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1312857110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gordon, I., Zagoory-Sharon, O., Schneiderman, I., Leckman, J. F., Weller, A., and Feldman, R. (2008). Oxytocin and cortisol in romantically unattached young adults: associations with bonding and psychological distress. Psychophysiology 45, 349–352. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00649.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grubaugh, A. L., Magruder, K. M., Waldrop, A. E., Elhai, J. D., Knapp, R. G., and Frueh, B. C. (2005). Subthreshold PTSD in primary care: prevalence, psychiatric disorders, healthcare use and functional status. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 193, 658–664. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000180740.02644.ab

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, J. P., Hayes, S. M., and Mikedis, A. M. (2012). Quantitative meta-analysis of neural activity in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Mood Anxiety Disord. 2:9. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-2-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinrichs, M., Baumgartner, T., Kirschbaum, C., and Ehlert, U. (2003). Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 1389–1398. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00465-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinkley, L. B. N., Vinogradov, S., Guggisberg, A. G., Fisher, M., Findlay, A. M., and Nagarajan, S. S. (2011). Clinical symptoms and alpha band resting-state functional connectivity imaging in patients with schizophrenia: implications for novel approaches to treatment. Biol. Psychiatry 70, 1134–1142. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.06.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, M.-X., Yurgil, K. A., Robb, A., Angeles, A., Diwakar, M., Risbrough, V. B., et al. (2014). Voxel-wise resting-state MEG source magnitude imaging study reveals neurocircuitry abnormality in active-duty service members and veterans with PTSD. Neuroimage Clin. 5, 408–419. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jakupcak, M., Conybeare, D., Phelps, L., Hunt, S., Holmes, H. A., Felker, B., et al. (2007). Anger, hostility and aggression among Iraq and Afghanistan war veterans reporting PTSD and subthreshold PTSD. J. Trauma. Stress 20, 945–954. doi: 10.1002/jts.20258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, O., and Mazaheri, A. (2010). Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity: gating by inhibition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:186. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jokić-begić, N., and Begić, D. (2003). Quantitative electroencephalogram (qEEG) in combat veterans with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Nord. J. Psychiatry 57, 351–355. doi: 10.1080/08039480310002688

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jurado, M. B., and Rosselli, M. (2007). The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding. Neuropsychol. Rev. 17, 213–233. doi: 10.1007/s11065-007-9040-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karl, A., Schaefer, M., Malta, L. S., Dörfel, D., Rohleder, N., and Werner, A. (2006). A meta-analysis of structural brain abnormalities in PTSD. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, 1004–1031. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., Sonnega, A., Bromet, E., Hughes, M., and Nelson, C. B. (1995). Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity survey. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 52, 1048–1060. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950240066012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirsch, P., Esslinger, C., Chen, Q., Mier, D., Lis, S., Siddhanti, S., et al. (2005). Oxytocin modulates neural circuitry for social cognition and fear in humans. J. Neurosci. 25, 11489–11493. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3984-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kluetsch, R. C., Ros, T., Théberge, J., Frewen, P. A., Calhoun, V. D., Schmahl, C., et al. (2014). Plastic modulation of PTSD resting-state networks and subjective wellbeing by EEG neurofeedback. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 130, 123–136. doi: 10.1111/acps.12229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knobloch, H. S., Charlet, A., Hoffmann, L. C., Eliava, M., Khrulev, S., Cetin, A. H., et al. (2012). Evoked axonal oxytocin release in the central amygdala attenuates fear response. Neuron 73, 553–566. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kolassa, I.-T., Wienbruch, C., Neuner, F., Schauer, M., Ruf, M., Odenwald, M., et al. (2007). Altered oscillatory brain dynamics after repeated traumatic stress. BMC Psychiatry 7:56. doi: 10.1186/1471-244x-7-56

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramer, K. M., Cushing, B. S., Carter, C. S., Wu, J., and Ottinger, M. A. (2004). Sex and species differences in plasma oxytocin using an enzyme immunoassay. Can. J. Zool. 82, 1194–1200. doi: 10.1139/z04-098

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Labuschagne, I., Phan, K. L., Wood, A., Angstadt, M., Chua, P., Heinrichs, M., et al. (2010). Oxytocin attenuates amygdala reactivity to fear in generalized social anxiety disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 2403–2413. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lanius, R. A., Williamson, P. C., Hopper, J., Densmore, M., Boksman, K., Gupta, M. A., et al. (2003). Recall of emotional states in posttraumatic stress disorder: an fMRI investigation. Biol. Psychiatry 53, 204–210. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01466-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laufs, H., Kleinschmidt, A., Beyerle, A., Eger, E., Salek-Haddadi, A., Preibisch, C., et al. (2003a). EEG-correlated fMRI of human alpha activity. Neuroimage 19, 1463–1476. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00286-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laufs, H., Krakow, K., Sterzer, P., Eger, E., Beyerle, A., Salek-Haddadi, A., et al. (2003b). Electroencephalographic signatures of attentional and cognitive default modes in spontaneous brain activity fluctuations at rest. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 100, 11053–11058. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1831638100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liberzon, I., Taylor, S. F., Amdur, R., Jung, T. D., Chamberlain, K. R., Minoshima, S., et al. (1999). Brain activation in PTSD in response to trauma-related stimuli. Biol. Psychiatry 45, 817–826. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00246-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Light, K. C., Grewen, K. M., Amico, J. A., Brownley, K. A., West, S. G., Hinderliter, A. L., et al. (2005). Oxytocinergic activity is linked to lower blood pressure and vascular resistance during stress in postmenopausal women on estrogen replacement. Horm. Behav. 47, 540–548. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindauer, R. J. L., Booij, J., Habraken, J. B. A., van Meijel, E. P. M., Uylings, H. B. M., Olff, M., et al. (2008). Effects of psychotherapy on regional cerebral blood flow during trauma imagery in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder: a randomized clinical trial. Psychol. Med. 38, 543–554. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDonald, K., and Feifel, D. (2013). Helping oxytocin deliver: considerations in the development of oxytocin-based therapeutics for brain disorders. Front. Neurosci. 7:35. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDonald, K., and Feifel, D. (2014). Oxytocin’s role in anxiety: a critical appraisal. Brain Res. 1580, 22–56. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.01.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCullough, M. E., Churchland, P. S., and Mendez, A. J. (2013). Problems with measuring peripheral oxytocin: can the data on oxytocin and human behavior be trusted? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 1485–1492. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.04.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menon, V. (2011). Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: a unifying triple network model. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 483–506. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Domes, G., Kirsch, P., and Heinrichs, M. (2011). Oxytocin and vasopressin in the human brain: social neuropeptides for translational medicine. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 524–538. doi: 10.1038/nrn3044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milad, M. R., Pitman, R. K., Ellis, C. B., Gold, A. L., Shin, L. M., Lasko, N. B., et al. (2009). Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 66, 1075–1082. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.06.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morey, R. A., Petty, C. M., Cooper, D. A., Labar, K. S., and McCarthy, G. (2008). Neural systems for executive and emotional processing are modulated by symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder in Iraq War veterans. Psychiatry Res. 162, 59–72. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nunez, P. L., Wingeier, B. M., and Silberstein, R. B. (2001). Spatial-temporal structures of human alpha rhythms: theory, microcurrent sources, multiscale measurements and global binding of local networks. Hum. Brain Mapp. 13, 125–164. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Rethinking feelings: an fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 1215–1229. doi: 10.1162/089892902760807212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E., and Schoffelen, J.-M. (2011). FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011:156869. doi: 10.1155/2011/156869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Patel, R., Spreng, R. N., Shin, L. M., and Girard, T. A. (2012). Neurocircuitry models of posttraumatic stress disorder and beyond: a meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 2130–2142. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peres, J. F. P., Newberg, A. B., Mercante, J. P., Simão, M., Albuquerque, V. E., Peres, M. J. P., et al. (2007). Cerebral blood flow changes during retrieval of traumatic memories before and after psychotherapy: a SPECT study. Psychol. Med. 37, 1481–1491. doi: 10.1017/s003329170700997x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, S., Slattery, D. A., Uschold-Schmidt, N., Reber, S. O., and Neumann, I. D. (2014). Dose-dependent effects of chronic central infusion of oxytocin on anxiety, oxytocin receptor binding and stress-related parameters in mice. Psychoneuroendocrinology 42, 225–236. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phan, K. L., Fitzgerald, D. A., Nathan, P. J., Moore, G. J., Uhde, T. W., and Tancer, M. E. (2005). Neural substrates for voluntary suppression of negative affect: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 210–219. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pincus, D., Kose, S., Arana, A., Johnson, K., Morgan, P. S., Borckardt, J., et al. (2010). Inverse effects of oxytocin on attributing mental activity to others in depressed and healthy subjects: a double-blind placebo controlled fMRI study. Front. Psychiatry 1:134. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2010.00134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitman, R. K., Orr, S. P., and Lasko, N. B. (1993). Effects of intranasal vasopressin and oxytocin on physiologic responding during personal combat imagery in Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res. 48, 107–117. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(93)90035-f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitman, R. K., Rasmusson, A. M., Koenen, K. C., Shin, L. M., Orr, S. P., Gilbertson, M. W., et al. (2012). Biological studies of post-traumatic stress disorder. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 769–787. doi: 10.1038/nrn3339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabinak, C. A., MacNamara, A., Kennedy, A. E., Angstadt, M., Stein, M. B., Liberzon, I., et al. (2014). Focal and aberrant prefrontal engagement during emotion regulation in veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Depress. Anxiety 31, 851–861. doi: 10.1002/da.22243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichle, M. E., and Mintun, M. A. (2006). Brain work and brain imaging. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 29, 449–476. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112819

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rauch, S. L., Shin, L. M., and Phelps, E. A. (2006). Neurocircuitry models of posttraumatic stress disorder and extinction: human neuroimaging research—past, present and future. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 376–382. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rauch, S. L., Whalen, P. J., Shin, L. M., McInerney, S. C., Macklin, M. L., Lasko, N. B., et al. (2000). Exaggerated amygdala response to masked facial stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study. Biol. Psychiatry 47, 769–776. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00828-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, L. K., Frueh, B. C., and Acierno, R. (2010). Prevalence estimates of combat-related post-traumatic stress disorder: critical review. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 44, 4–19. doi: 10.3109/00048670903393597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, S. E., and Vrba, J. (1999). “Functional neuroimaging by synthetic aperture magnetometry,” in Recent Advances in Biomagnetism : Proceedings From the 11th International Conference on Biomagnetism, eds T. Yoshimine, M. Kotani, S. Kuriki, H. Karibe, and N. Nakasato (Sendai: Tokoku University Press), 302–305.

Google Scholar

Schnurr, P. P., Ford, J. D., Friedman, M. J., Green, B. L., Dain, B. J., and Sengupta, A. (2000). Predictors and outcomes of posttraumatic stress disorder in World War II veterans exposed to mustard gas. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 68, 258–268. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.68.2.258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnurr, P. P., Lunney, C. A., Bovin, M. J., and Marx, B. P. (2009). Posttraumatic stress disorder and quality of life: extension of findings to veterans of the wars in Iraq and Afghanistan. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 29, 727–735. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shalvi, S., and De Dreu, C. K. W. (2014). Oxytocin promotes group-serving dishonesty. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 111, 5503–5507. doi: 10.3410/f.718339864.793493695

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shin, L. M., Orr, S. P., Carson, M. A., Rauch, S. L., Macklin, M. L., Lasko, N. B., et al. (2004). Regional cerebral blood flow in the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex during traumatic imagery in male and female Vietnam veterans with PTSD. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 61, 168–176. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.2.168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shin, L. M., Whalen, P. J., Pitman, R. K., Bush, G., Macklin, M. L., Lasko, N. B., et al. (2001). An fMRI study of anterior cingulate function in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 50, 932–942. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01215-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shin, L. M., Wright, C. I., Cannistraro, P. A., Wedig, M. M., McMullin, K., Martis, B., et al. (2005). A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to overtly presented fearful faces in posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 273–281. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.3.273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simmons, A. N., Matthews, S. C., Strigo, I. A., Baker, D. G., Donovan, H. K., Motezadi, A., et al. (2011). Altered amygdala activation during face processing in Iraqi and Afghanistani war veterans. Biol. Mood Anxiety Disord. 1:6. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-1-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, T. C., Ryan, M. A. K., Wingard, D. L., Slymen, D. J., Sallis, J. F., and Kritz-Silverstein, D. (2008). New onset and persistent symptoms of post-traumatic stress disorder self reported after deployment and combat exposures: prospective population based US military cohort study. BMJ 336, 366–371. doi: 10.1136/bmj.39430.638241.ae

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sripada, R. K., King, A. P., Garfinkel, S. N., Wang, X., Sripada, C. S., Welsh, R. C., et al. (2012). Altered resting-state amygdala functional connectivity in men with posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 37, 241–249. doi: 10.1503/jpn.110069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sripada, C. S., Phan, K. L., Labuschagne, I., Welsh, R., Nathan, P. J., and Wood, A. G. (2013). Oxytocin enhances resting-state connectivity between amygdala and medial frontal cortex. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 16, 255–260. doi: 10.1017/s1461145712000533

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, J. S., Jovanovic, T., Fani, N., Ely, T. D., Glover, E. M., Bradley, B., et al. (2013). Disrupted amygdala-prefrontal functional connectivity in civilian women with posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 47, 1469–1478. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.05.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tal, I., and Abeles, M. (2013). Cleaning MEG artifacts using external cues. J. Neurosci. Methods 217, 31–38. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tavanti, M., Battaglini, M., Borgogni, F., Bossini, L., Calossi, S., Marino, D., et al. (2012). Evidence of diffuse damage in frontal and occipital cortex in the brain of patients with post-traumatic stress disorder. Neurol. Sci. 33, 59–68. doi: 10.1007/s10072-011-0659-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomaes, K., Dorrepaal, E., Draijer, N., Jansma, E. P., Veltman, D. J., and van Balkom, A. J. (2014). Can pharmacological and psychological treatment change brain structure and function in PTSD? A systematic review. J. Psychiatr. Res. 50, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vasterling, J. J., Brailey, K., Constans, J. I., and Sutker, P. B. (1998). Attention and memory dysfunction in posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychology 12, 125–133. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.12.1.125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viviani, D., Charlet, A., van den Burg, E., Robinet, C., Hurni, N., Abatis, M., et al. (2011). Oxytocin selectively gates fear responses through distinct outputs from the central amygdala. Science 333, 104–107. doi: 10.1126/science.1201043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watts, B. V., Landon, B., Groft, A., and Young-Xu, Y. (2012). A sham controlled study of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for posttraumatic stress disorder. Brain Stimulat. 5, 38–43. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2011.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weisman, O., and Feldman, R. (2013). Oxytocin effects on the human brain: findings, questions and future directions. Biol. Psychiatry 74, 158–159. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.05.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weisman, O., Schneiderman, I., Zagoory-Sharon, O., and Feldman, R. (2013). Salivary vasopressin increases following intranasal oxytocin administration. Peptides 40, 99–103. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2012.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, L. M., Kemp, A. H., Felmingham, K., Barton, M., Olivieri, G., Peduto, A., et al. (2006). Trauma modulates amygdala and medial prefrontal responses to consciously attended fear. Neuroimage 29, 347–357. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.03.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Windle, R. J., Kershaw, Y. M., Shanks, N., Wood, S. A., Lightman, S. L., and Ingram, C. D. (2004). Oxytocin attenuates stress-induced c-fos mRNA expression in specific forebrain regions associated with modulation of hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal activity. J. Neurosci. 24, 2974–2982. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3432-03.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yan, X., Brown, A. D., Lazar, M., Cressman, V. L., Henn-Haase, C., Neylan, T. C., et al. (2013). Spontaneous brain activity in combat related PTSD. Neurosci. Lett. 547, 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.04.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yatzkar, U., and Klein, E. (2010). Intranasal oxytocin in patients with post traumatic stress disorder: a single dose, pilot double blind crossover study. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 20:S84. doi: 10.1016/s0924-977x(10)70098-5

CrossRef Full Text

Yehuda, R. (2002). Post-traumatic stress disorder. N. Engl. J. Med. 346, 108–114. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra012941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: oxytocin, MEG, alpha oscillations, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), PTSD, veterans

Citation: Eidelman-Rothman M, Goldstein A, Levy J, Weisman O, Schneiderman I, Mankuta D, Zagoory-Sharon O and Feldman R (2015) Oxytocin affects spontaneous neural oscillations in trauma-exposed war veterans. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:165. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00165

Received: 01 April 2015; Accepted: 15 June 2015;
Published: 29 June 2015.

Edited by:

Regina Marie Sullivan, Nathan Kline Institute & NYU School of Medicine, USA

Reviewed by:

Richard Paul Ebstein, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Israel
Bianca Jones Marlin, New York University, USA

Copyright © 2015 Eidelman-Rothman, Goldstein, Levy, Weisman, Schneiderman, Mankuta, Zagoory-Sharon and Feldman. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ruth Feldman, The Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center and Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan University, Gonda Brain Sciences, Building 901, Ramat-Gan 52900, Israel, feldman@mail.biu.ac.il